Foto: Marion Fayolle |
Google is
good at finding information, but the brain beats it in two essential ways. Champions
of Google underestimate how much the meaning of words and sentences changes
with context. Consider vocabulary. Every teacher knows that a sixth grader,
armed with a thesaurus, will often submit a paper studded with words used in
not-quite-correct ways, like the student who looked up “meticulous,” saw it
meant “very careful,” and wrote “I was meticulous when I fell off the cliff.”
With the
right knowledge in memory, your brain deftly puts words in context. Consider
“Trisha spilled her coffee.” When followed by the sentence “Dan jumped up to
get a rag,” the brain instantly highlights one aspect of the meaning of “spill”
— spills make a mess. Had the second sentence been “Dan jumped up to get her
more,” you would have thought instead of the fact that “spill” means Trisha had
less of something. Still another aspect of meaning would come to mind had you
read, “Dan jumped up, howling in pain.”
The meaning
of “spill” depends on context, but dictionaries, including internet
dictionaries, necessarily offer context-free meanings. That’s why kids fall off
cliffs meticulously.
Perhaps
internet searches will become more sensitive to context, but until our brains
communicate directly with silicon chips, there’s another problem — speed.
Quick
access is supposed to be a great advantage of using the internet. Students have
always been able to look up the quadratic equation rather than memorize it, but
opening a new browser tab takes moments, not the minutes required to locate the
right page in the right book. Yet “moments” is still much slower than the brain
operates.
Every
weekday, get thought-provoking commentary from Op-Ed columnists, the Times
editorial board and contributing writers from around the world.
You agree
to receive occasional updates and special offers for The New York Times's
products and services.
Speed
matters when the quadratic equation is part of a larger problem. Imagine
solving 397,394 x 9 if you hadn’t memorized the multiplication table. Sure, you
could look up 4 x 9, but you could easily lose the thread of the problem as you
did so. That’s why the National Mathematics Advisory Panel listed “quick and
effortless recall of facts” as one essential of math education.
Speed
matters for reading, too. Researchers report that readers need to know at least
95 percent of the words in a text for comfortable absorption. Pausing to find a
word definition is disruptive. Online, the mere presence of hyperlinks
compromises reading comprehension because the decision of whether or not to
click disrupts the flow of understanding.
Deeper
knowledge of words also helps. Your knowledge of what a word means, how it’s
spelled and how it sounds are actually separate in the brain. That’s why you
may recall one but not the others, as when you know what you want to say
(“someone who owes money”) but can’t find the word (“debtor”). Good readers
have reliable, speedy connections among the brain representations of spelling,
sound and meaning. Speed matters because it allows other important work — for
example, puzzling out the meaning of phrases — to proceed.
Using
knowledge in the head is also self-sustaining, whereas using knowledge from the
internet is not. Every time you retrieve information from memory, it becomes a
bit easier to find it the next time. That’s why students studying for a test
actually remember more if they quiz themselves than if they study as they
typically do, by rereading their textbook or notes. That parades the right
ideas before the mind, but doesn’t make them stick. In the same way, you won’t
learn your way around a city if you always use your GPS, but you will if you
work to remember the route you took last time.
The brain
beats the internet when it comes to context and speed, but the internet
clobbers the brain when it comes to volume. You can find any fact on the
internet, even alternative ones. Your brain, in contrast, is limited, so how
should we choose what to learn?
Students
should learn the information for which the internet is a poor substitute. Getting
information from the internet takes time, so they should memorize facts that
are needed fast and frequently. Elementary math facts and the sounds of letters
are obvious choices, but any information that is needed with high frequency is
a candidate — in algebra, that’s the quadratic equation.
The
internet is poor at putting information in context. Kids who look up the
quadratic equation may end up like the child who looked up “meticulous”; they
have a definition, but they don’t have the background knowledge to use it
correctly. Students should learn not only the formula but also why it works and
how it connects to other math content. That’s how contextual knowledge develops
in the brain, and that’s why vocabulary instruction seldom consists of simple
memorization of definitions — students are asked to use the words in a variety
of sentences. The same should be true of more advanced concepts and for the
same reason.
No hay comentarios:
Publicar un comentario